Site icon CureJoy

The Only Child Syndrome: Do Siblings Hinder Creativity?

“Being an only child is a disease in itself,“ said Granville Stanley Hall, a pioneer in child psychology.

This is a common perception, or should we say misconception, not only among acquaintances of only children but among the scientific community as well. The only child has often been called an oddball and permanent misfit – descriptions that have trickled down from the 1896 study called Of Peculiar and Exceptional Children. Simply put, onlies are often misconstrued as the not-so-friendly type.

Advertisements

It is important to note that a number of studies have opposed this statement, suggesting no difference in sociability on account of the presence or absence of siblings.1

An Only Child Has More Time And Resources To Develop Intellect

Advertisements

Let’s bring to light the advantage of belonging to the singleton niche. Parents can focus all of their time and money for the development of one child without having to divide them among other children. Resource dilution or the sharing of parents’ resources does not exist if you are their only heir.

Because of this, kids with no siblings often show greater inclination to indulge in intellectual and artistic activities.2 They’re very comfortable with alone time, and even prefer it, giving them plenty of scope to explore their creative, thinking side.

Advertisements

Looking to delve deeper into understanding the impact of growing up with siblings, a research group in China conducted a study.

Perceptions About Only Children May Have A Neural Basis

Advertisements

A study was done to find out whether differences in intelligence, creativity, and personality traits between only children and those with siblings can be traced back to differences in brain structures.3 Yes, it can. Gray matter volumes in parts of the brain responsible for these qualities were different for singleton kids compared to those with siblings.

Only Children Are More Creative But Less Compliant

Advertisements

The study was done on 250 university students. About half of them were an only child. Standard IQ, creativity, and personality tests were carried out.

In agreement with previous social assumptions and hypotheses, higher levels of flexibility (as a measure of creativity) and lower levels of agreeableness (as a dimension of personality type) were found in only children in comparison to those with siblings.

Advertisements

So, do siblings hinder creativity? According to this study, they possibly do.

Volumes Of Gray Matter In Only Children Are Different From Those With Siblings

In addition, brain scans were done on the test subjects to measure the volumes of gray matter in differrent parts of the brain. The absence of siblings did affect the parts of the brain responsible for creativity and agreeableness. Differences were seen in the following brain structures:

Advertisements

As is evident, the brain structures responsible for flexibility and agreeableness were affected.

The Caveat: The Study Focused On One Component Of Creativity

While the study suggests the influence of family size on the architecture of our brain, there are loopholes:

The 3 components are: originality (how rare an idea is), flexibility (how many types of ideas are generated), and fluency (how many well-interpreted, relevant ideas are expressed). Originality is a more convincing indicator for creativity and outweighs flexibility. So, the conclusion about creativity in only children is not completely reliable.

That said, this study open doors for more comprehensive research on how siblings help mould our adult personalities.

References[+]

References
1, 2 Trent, Katherine, and Glenna Spitze. “Growing up without siblings and adult sociability behaviors.” Journal of family issues 32, no. 9 (2011): 1178-1204.
3 Yang, Junyi, Xin Hou, Dongtao Wei, Kangcheng Wang, Yadan Li, and Jiang Qiu. “Only-child and non-only-child exhibit differences in creativity and agreeableness: evidence from behavioral and anatomical structural studies.” Brain imaging and behavior (2016): 1-10.
Exit mobile version